The Parable of the
Minimal Groups
Psychology studies
continually reinforce the notion that humans will form groups in ways they are
not consciously aware of, and with very little (minimal) external prompting.
For instance, in a famous study, participants are brought into a room and given
some small task to perform. When they enter the room each participant is given
a pen, either a red pen or a blue pen. After filling out some questionnaires,
the participants are done. It’s common in psychology experiments like this one
to pay participants. But in this experiment a surprising thing happens. When
one person is given a bundle of cash and told to distribute the money to the
other participants, that person does not always distribute the money equally.
Instead, it is common that the distributor will give out more money to those
with the same color pen as he or she has. If she was given a blue pen, she’ll
give more money to people with blue pens. If she was given a red pen, she’ll
give more money to people with red pens. This is called a minimal grouping:
we form groups, in ways that have real consequences, based on very, very thin
information. Why do we do this? It seems that humans are instinctively quite
adept at parsing the world into recognizable and manageable parcels of
information. When there is no obviously meaningful way to distinguish among
random strangers, we will begin distinguishing people based on arbitrary
characteristics. Of course, we all know what can happen (what does happen) when we divide the world
into arbitrary groups, assigning more worth to some than others: racism,
sexism, and other forms of prejudice and discrimination. The problem of
discrimination is precisely this: the confusion of an arbitrary
difference with a meaningful one. We know that dividing the world into in-groups
and out-groups is hard to avoid, but we know the consequences are profound. So,
how do we keep ourselves from doing it?
What is Race?
Race is a fiction,
the grouping of individuals by perceived physical characteristics, or
phenotypes. We often imagine that race is inherited by a biological or
blood-borne factor, but physical groupings of individuals are chosen, and are not inevitable. What we
call “race” is socially constructed. The lines between groups are not
distinct, are not biologically meaningful, are not universally recognized, and
are instead the product of historical, economic, and cultural preferences (and,
indeed, of historical, economic, and cultural mistakes). In other words, racial
groupings are arbitrary groupings.
Having said all
that, race remains socially powerful. We continue to define ourselves and our
neighbors by race, and we often acknowledge that our “race” is an important
part of our identity. Race may indeed be (and indeed is) a social construction,
but it is small consolation to victims of prejudice to be told that race is
just a fiction.
Effects of
Prejudice
Prejudice is alive
and well in a whole host of ways in our lives today – as I know you are all
well aware. I hope you are also aware of the distinction between implicit and
explicit prejudice. Explicit prejudice occurs as a conscious act, while
implicit prejudice is generally unconscious. Implicit prejudice occurs either
because people are unaware of their own biases, or because institutions – like
governments, schools, or workplaces – have codified practices that perpetuate
inequality, even when no one individual is aware of the problem. I will give you just one brief example of an
institutional practice responsible for perpetuating bias (though I expect a lot
of you are aware of others). When you started classes at this school, most of
you took the Accuplacer Test, a fairly innocuous standardized exam that places
you into your writing and mathematics courses. You may remember that before you
began answering the exam questions, you were asked to fill out demographic
information: questions about your gender, race, and so on. There are lots of
good reasons for schools like North Hennepin to ask students demographic
questions. In particular, the school wants to know who is taking classes, and
who is succeeding. If we have more
women graduating than men, or more white faces graduating than dark faces, that
is an inequality which the school will want to correct. And as an aside,
Minnesota high schools have one of the most pronounced “opportunity gaps” in
the nation. This means that the gap between the number of white kids graduating
and the number of black kids graduating (or of native kids or Latino kids
graduating, etc.) is quite pronounced. And that signals a problem. We do not
want “race” to be a predictor of high school graduation rates. Nevertheless, in
Minnesota, it is.
But back to the
Accuplacer Test. The problem is not with the test per se, the weirdness arises
from asking the demographic questions before
students take the exam. What effect do you think that has on the test results?
Black kids do worse than they otherwise would, and white kids do better than
they otherwise would (and, to be clear, these trends are generalizations – it
doesn’t necessarily mean that you
experienced this effect). Why does this happen? It happens because when
students are asked to reveal details about their race, gender, etc., they are
rendered both invisible and hypervisible. Invisible because they feel they are not seen for who they really
are, but instead as an anonymous member of a stereotyped group, and
hypervisible because they feel they’ve been seen, or are being watched. As a
result, the increased anxiety sabotages student performance.
The point of
bringing this up is not to focus on the Accuplacer Test in particular, but to
note it as but one example in which our unconscious expectations about race and
identity deeply affect how we think, how we act, and how we regard and treat
others. In other words, when surrounded by institutional racism, we begin to
think differently, even about ourselves.
Another aside: the
greatest predictor of whether you
will graduate from North Hennepin is not race, though it overlaps with race,
gender, and other “group” information. The greatest predictor of whether you
will graduate is simply whether you identify with the people at the school. If
you feel that there are people at the school who identify with you, recognize
you, and understand you, you’re much more likely to stay invested in school. On
the other hand, if you feel that at school no one is really like you, or if you
feel that you have to change who you
are in order to succeed in school, you’ll lose motivation. So, make sure you
find people at school, whether teachers, staff, or students, who you identify
with and who understand you. Now that
classes are entirely online, this will be harder to do, but try to make that
connection - your success may hinge on it!
What are we to do?
An astonishing set
of recent experiments shed light on prejudice – in rats. Rats, it turns out,
have an acute sense of empathy. In an experiment, when rats are given a choice
between having a piece of chocolate (a treat they really like) or saving a
friend who is drowning, rats will regularly skip the chocolate in order to save
the drowning friend (as an aside, rats will save their fellows even more reliably than humans will. I
myself have literally seen humans refuse to save a fellow drowning human). Now,
the experimental condition just described can be altered in interesting ways.
When a white rat who has been raised with other white rats sees a black rat drowning, what will she do? She’ll
choose to eat the chocolate instead of saving the drowning rat. She doesn’t
recognize the drowning friend. However, when a white rat is raised with both
white and black rats, and is faced with saving a black rat, what will she do?
She’ll save her friend, of course, because she recognizes her. Now for the
really interesting condition: when a white rat is raised with only black rats,
and is asked to save a white rat,
what will she do? She’ll eat the chocolate! In other words, the rat does not
recognize a rat that looks like herself.
Well, what does this
tell us about discrimination? It tells that who
we identify with is malleable. It may be inevitable that we divide
information into categories, but it is not
inevitable that we divide people along arbitrary lines. Rats will save rats who
they identify as part of their in-group, so, obviously, we need to learn to
recognize one another as members of the same group, with the same basic
humanity (just as the rats can learn to recognize a basic rodentity!). How do
we do this? Actually, it’s really really easy. Through mere exposure
people will come to recognize one’s another’s shared interests and shared
humanity. Mere exposure just means exposing people to one another, just as in a
class, with a lot of people from all kinds of different backgrounds. Once
people spend some time with one another, they’re much more likely to skip the
chocolate and save one another when drowning. Or, to put it more precisely,
you’re much more likely to skip the chocolate and save a person who looks like one of your classmates![1]
Now, mere exposure doesn’t always work; it works under one particular
circumstance. Mere exposure works when people meet in equality. A
classroom is the perfect example: you’re all equal in the classroom, all
subject to the same conditions, all suffering through my lectures. Under such
conditions, you’ll broaden the scope of who you have empathy for. But notice
that when people meet in inequality, the opposite happens. Imagine a town with
two distinct populations, one a minority, one a majority, one with power, and
one without power. Will these people come to have empathy with each other? No,
just the opposite. Even though they often see each other, they’ll come to see
each group as even more distinct, and
will come to judge one another with disdain – and a look at south Minneapolis,
where George Floyd was killed, reinforces the truth of this effect. Power
differential matters! So, mere exposure will broaden people’s empathy, but only
exposure under conditions of equality.
Get Free Quote!
426 Experts Online